02-General Theories on Second Language Acquisition and Learning

General theories on Second language Acquisition and Learning. The Concept of Interlanguage. The treatment of Error. 1.

Views 240 Downloads 10 File size 110KB

Report DMCA / Copyright

DOWNLOAD FILE

Recommend stories

Citation preview

General theories on Second language Acquisition and Learning. The Concept of Interlanguage. The treatment of Error.

1. Introduction Language acquisition is one of the most impressive and fascinating aspects of human development. We listen with pleasure to the “coos” and “gurgles” of a three-month-old baby. We laugh and “answer” the conversational “ba-ba-ba” babbling of older babies, and we share in the pride and joy of parents whose oneyear-old has uttered the first “mommy” or “daddy”. Indeed, learning a language is an amazing feat, one which has attracted the attention of linguists and psychologists for generations. How do children accomplish this? On the other hand how do adults acquire a second or foreign language? The answer to these questions helps to solve one each other and they will be the subject of the main part of this paper.

2. General Theories of Second Language Learning and Acquisition 2.1. Introduction Theories of Second Language Acquisition (SLA) range a continuum from nativist through interactionist to environmentalist. They differ in the relative importance they attach to innate mechanisms and knowledge, to interactions among innate abilities, learned abilities and environmental factors. 2.2. Environmentalist Theories of SLA 2.2.1. General Characteristics Environmentalist theories of learning hold that an organism’s experience is of more importance to development than its innate contributions. They deny that innate contributions play any other role than providing the animal with the internal structure which environmental factor can proceed to shape. The best known examples are those behaviourist’s Stimulus-Response learning theories which enhance the position of an innate knowledge which is conveniently adapted and shaped according to the stimuli that it receives from the environment. Skinner

developed some theories in his Verbal Behaviour of 1957 which were well noted, but which were soon overwhelmed by Chomsky´s revision of them in 1959. In our role of English teachers we have to say that the S-R models little can contribute to the explanation of a SLA, but a related family of connectionist models have excited considerable interest in some of their sources to be used for the development of diverse areas namely motor control, visual perception, and memory. Of particular interest for language learning theory is the work of McClelland et all of 1986 on Parallel Distributed Processing (PDP). PDP is a theory of cognition which assumes no innate endowment. They say that the learning is based on processing of input, but do not believe that the input processing results in the accrual of rules, but something much more spontaneous. While it is arguable that no pure environmentalist theories of language learning had been advanced in recent years, some other ones have been developed and indirectly they have fallen into environmental fields of knowledge when regarding external input for explaining language acquisition. Among the strongest ones of such theories is Schumann´s attempt to account for naturalistic SLA as a by-product of acculturation. 2.2.2. The Acculturation Model Schumann holds that the degree of elaboration observed in later stages of interlanguage (IL) development is a function of social and psychological variables that initially produces pidginization, which is a mixture of two languages. And collectively he makes up a large causal factor in SLA, acculturation, roughly translatable as “the process of becoming adapted to a new culture”. He mentions Social Distance as a group-level phenomenon consisting of eight factors whose values seem negative: 1. Social Dominance: the fact that a learner is member of a social group which is subordinate to the TL group interferes his/her assimilation in it. 2. Integration pattern: in case in which the learner is reluctant to adapt his own cultural identity.

3. Enclosure: when the learner belong to a higher enclosure group. 4. Cohesiveness: if a learner belongs to a fairly cohesive group, this tends to mitigate contact with the TL group. 5. Size: Being a member of a fairly large group tends to facilitate intra-group contact, rather than inter-group contact. 6. Cultural Congruence: if the learner belongs to a group which is not congruent with TL group. 7. Attitude: inter-group attitudes can shift from neutral to hostile. 8. Intended length of residence: The shorter the stay, the less likely it is for extensive contacts with the TL group to be developed. Schumann differentiates among two types of acculturation. In Type One acculturation, learners are psychologically open to the TL. The first factor means that they have enough contacts with speakers for them to acquire the second language. In Type Two acculturation, learners are socially integrated and psychologically open, but also consciously or unconsciously wish to adopt the lifestyle and values of the TL group. Either type of acculturation is sufficient to ensure SLA. Now we will see a different approach to SLA through the Nativist Theories which have been developed being Chomsky the most important name to be regarded within these theories. 2.3. Nativist Theories of SLA. 2.3.1. General Characteristics Nativist theories are those which purport to explain acquisition by positing an innate biological endowment that makes learning possible. Chomsky (1965), for example, posits innate knowledge of substantive universals such as syntactic categories (subject, object, noun, and verb) and distinctive phonological features, and formal universals (abstract principles governing possible rules and parameters of human languages). In still other nativist theories, the innate endowment involves both linguistic principles and general cognitive notions. Chomsky´s various theories

of child language development (1965, 1980, 1981) are the best known nativist claims, and some of his ideas have been invoked (often critically) in SLA theory construction as well. 2.3.2. Chomsky’s Universal Grammar and SLA Chomsky as noted above claims the idea that humans are innately endowed with universal language-specific knowledge, that is called by him Universal Grammar (UG). Without such endowment language learning (first or second) would be impossible because the input data are insufficiently “rich” to allow acquisition ever to occur. Chomskyan view of the input is deficient or “poor” in two ways. It is claimed to be “degenerate” in the sense that it is marred by performance features, such as false starts, slips, fragments, and ungrammatically resulting from these and other pressures inherent in real time communication which constitute inadequate data base for language learning. Some other senses have been considered to regard input degenerate, for example when regarding first language acquisition children´s utterances is full of mistakes and they rarely are grammatically corrected. The same happens with second language acquisition when native speakers do not monitor learners of that second language when using language inappropriately in conversation. Since this lack of correction learners of a language are unavailable to find out when they have committed a mistake when using language. Chomsky talks about some principles and parameters innately endowed which are responsible of correcting the settings triggered by the input. They constitute what Chomsky calls the “core grammar” and it would be implemented by some rules developed through experience. Krashen offers another view on nativist theories through his monitor theory. 2.3.3. Krashen Monitor´s Theory One of the best known and most influential theories of SLA in the early 1970s and early 1980s was Krashen´s Monitor Theory (MT). It was started not as a theory of Second Language Acquisition but as an attempt of a model of SL performance. He took into account two phenomena, first a generalization was

emerging from the “morpheme studies” that there existed statistically significant associations between the orders of appearance of certain English grammatical morphemes, accurately supplied in obligatory contexts, in the speech and writing of SL learners of different ages. And second he observed some disturbances in the natural order of acquisition of SL learners, specifically in the reading and writing levels, which were common to most of them. Krashen stated that two separate knowledge systems underlay SL performance. The acquired system which is a subconscious knowledge that native speakers have of their own language (this happens when a native speaker of a language says that a grammatical construction is correct or not, but he or she does not know to explain why) that little by little it becomes adapted to SL, and the learned system which is the product of formal instruction (typically classroom language teaching) and comprises conscious knowledge of “easy” SL grammatical rules, such as those for subject verb agreement, pluralizing NPs, etc. The acquired system was typically the only knowledge source that speakers could use in real time communication, when they were attending to meaning, not to form; the learned system served only as a planner and editor with which to inspect, or monitor, the output of the acquired system. The learned system was only accessible when three conditions were met: there was time, the learner was focused on form, and obviously the learner knew the rule. The “natural order”, Krashen held, was the surface manifestation of the acquired system; disturbed orders were caused by by (“big M”) Monitoring intrusion of the learned system on performance tasks which encouraged its use, with the result that certain morphemes governed by low-level grammar rules, like the third-person singular –s, regular past –ed and plural –s, were supplied more accurately. Krashen claimed that (“small m”) monitoring, self-correcting using conscious or subconscious general awareness of language could be done by “feel” judgements, using the acquired system.

The two systems like the processes that produced them, acquisition and learning, operated separately. In a series of books and papers appearing between 1978 and 1985, the Monitor model underwent a number of modifications. It became a “theory”, Monitor Theory (MT), of (child and adult, naturalistic and instructed) SLA. There were as many as ten “hypotheses” in the early 1980s, reduced to five major claims in more recent formulations: 1) The Acquisition-Learning Hypothesis It states that there are two independent ways of learning a SL acquisition and learning, both technical terms in MT: “acquisition” refers to the subconscious process used by children developing their fist language; so SLA is similar to L1 acquisition. Errors are accepted. “Learning” is a conscious process, it is not responsible for our fluency, but for editing or monitoring our linguistic performance. 2) The Natural Order Hypothesis SL rules are acquired in a predictable order, one apparently not determined solely by linguistic complexity, and certainly not by the order in which the items appear in teaching syllabuses. 3) The Monitor Hypothesis Regarding the relationship posited between the acquired and the learned systems during SL performance, the former is the utterance initiator, while the latter acts in planning, editing and correcting when the three mentioned conditions are met. SL learners can be successful if monitoring is used for grammar tests and writing; overusers, however, may have difficulty in acquiring fluency. 4) The Input Hypotheses SL is acquired through processing comprehensible input (CI), that is, language that is heard or read and understood. Language which is not understood does not help. Not all CI helps, either; understanding

messages in a SL just because they are encoded in target-language samples which are less complex than the learner is already capable of dealing with may help the ego, but not to lead to interlanguage improvement. Progress along the “natural order” is achieved when a learner receives CI that contains in principle more information that the one that he is presumed to process according to his learning characteristics, however he process all the information and adds a new notion to this input base. That is called I+1, that is, structures which are one step beyond to the current stage of the speaker. Those structures which are unknown to the learner are understood through the help of linguistic and extralinguistic content. This fact would be improved by Chomsky´s Language Acquisition Device (LAD) that he would define as an internal language processor that provides the speaker with the necessary data when the input has been understood to give appropriate feedback. 5) The Affective Filter Hypothesis It embodies Krashen´s views that various affective factors, including motivation, self-confidence and lack of anxiety, play a facilitative, but non-casual, role in SLA. Lack of motivation, low self-esteem, debilitating anxiety, and so on can combine to “raise the filter”, to form a “mental block”, which prevents CI from reaching the LAD Krashen states that people acquire second languages only if they obtain comprehensible input and if their affective filters are low enough to allow the input “in”. When the filter is “down” and appropriate comprehensible input is presented (and comprehended), acquisition is inevitable. It is, in fact, unavoidable and cannot be prevented. He continues with the assumption that the language as a “mental organ” will function just as automatically as any another organ.

Krashen theories have been widely used in methods for Second Language

teaching, some of the aspects used which are derived

from them are: -A focus on meaning and not form, by teacher and students at all times. - Proscription of structural grading and error correction. -Creation of a positive affective classroom climate in order to “low the filter”. 2.4. Interactional Theories of SLA 2.4.1. General Characteristics Interactional theories are the most powerful ones since they invoke both innate and environmental factors. They say that the issue of language learning is too complex to be explained with nativist or environmentalist factors alone. Interactionist theories of SLA differ greatly from one another. Some, such as Givon ´s Functional-Typological Theory, originate in functional-typological syntax and diachronic language change. Others are partly inspired by experimental psycholinguistics and cognitive psychology. Others are partly inspired by experimental psycholinguistics and cognitive psychology. Still others draw on social, cognitive and linguistic theory and on findings from the discourse analyses of first and second language acquisition to study and explain SLA. 2.4.2. Givon´s Functional-Typological Theory and SLA Givon´s functional-typological syntactic analysis (FTSA) is functionalist in its view that syntax emanates from properties of human discourse; that is, syntax has not been issued separate from language use, it is one of the features of language that makes it to be understood; and typological in its consideration of a diverse body of languages, not simply a single language of language family. It was first used for the study of diachronic syntax, to rate the level syntax was in a constant flux of change according to the different users of a language over time.

Givon claims, speakers and linguistic systems move from a discourse-based, pragmatic mode of communication to a more syntactic mode. This process of syntacticization operates over a number of features which are contrasted across the pragmatic and syntactic modes of communication. 2.4.3. The Zisa Group´s Multidimensional Model The Zisa´s project, one of the most important bodies of SLA research, was carried out in the University of Hamburg in the late 1970s by using interview data to study the naturalistic acquisition of German as a SL (GSL) by speakers of Spanish and Italian. It regards several stages. After an initial period during which learner production consisted of isolated words and formulae they were adhered to a developmental sequence stage. Learners did not abandon one IL rule for the next as they traversed the sequence, but they accumulated rules, adding new ones and retaining the old. They used some combinations of speech-processing strategies which can be used to explain the developmental stages in the word-order data, to which they are hierarchically related, such that each new one entails and adds to the sophistication of the previous one, thereby gradually allowing the processing of psycholinguistically more complex structures. The stages are by definition statements about what a learner can be taught at a given step of development. Another application is to teaching methodology, where knowledge of developmental stages and variational features can serve as a useful diagnostic resource for teachers, allowing them to identify different kinds of errors and to asses their “remediability”.

3.

The Concept of Interlanguage

3.1. Definitions of Interlanguage The concept of interlanguage is used in Second Language Acquisition research to refer to the systematic knowledge of a language which is different from both the learner´s L1 and the L2 system he is trying to learn. This research indicated that there were strong similarities in the developmental route followed by

different L2 teachers. As a result of this research, it was suggested that SLA followed a “universal” route that was largely uninfluenced by such factors as the age of the learner, the context in which language took place, or the learner´s L1 background. The term interlanguage was first used by Selinker (1972). First interlanguage refers to the structured system which the learner constructs at any given stage in his development. The assumptions underlying interlanguage theory were stated clearly by Nemser (1971). - At any given time the approximative system is distinct from L1 and L2 - The approximate systems form an evolving series - That in a given contact situation, the approximative systems of learners at the same stage of proficiency roughly coincide. The concept of “hypothesis-testing” was created by Corder (1967) he suggested that both L1 and L2 learners make errors in order to test out certain hypotheses about the nature of the language they are learning. Corder saw the making of errors as a strategy, evidence of learner-internal processing. “Hypothesis-testing” was a mentalist notion and had no place in behaviourist accounts of learning, where imitation and repetition where the key to learning. Selinker (1972) suggested that five principal processes operated in interlanguage. These were: 1) Language transfer 2) Overgeneralization of the target language rules 3) Transfer of training 4) Strategies of L2 learning 5) Strategies of L2 communication Selinker also noted that many L2 learners fail to reach target language competence. They stop learning when their interlanguage contains at least some rules different from those of the target language system which are apprehended as

patterns. He referred to this as fossilization. Fossilization come to occur when the learner thinks that he does not need his interlanguage any further in order to communicate effectively whatever he wants to, or it can occur because changes in the neural structure of his brain as a result of age restrict the operation of the hypothesis-testing mechanisms. The structures that comprehended speakers´ interlanguage at a certain stage become constant have no room to be implemented because of inner resistance of the speaker; this resistance can take place consciously or unconsciously. 3.2. Acquisition Orders and Developmental Sequence Interlanguages are variable, they are also systematic. And most of them presuppose an innate language-specific endowment. Krashen postulated a “natural order” for ESL (English as a Second Language, a concept different from EFL-English as a Foreign Language-is the label that English carries when it is spoken by non-native speakers in contexts where English is the partial or universal language). An acquisition order, therefore, is the order in which linguistics forms, rules, and items are acquired in the first-or secondlanguage learning. According to Krashen the order of acquisition is the following in English: 1. –ING, PLURAL, COPULA are the morphemes acquired first 2. AUXILIARY, ARTICLE come next 3. IRREGULAR PAST follows 4. REGULAR PAST, 3RD. SINGULAR, POSSESSIVE CASE are acquired much later. All these concerns define the existence of a developmental sequence which consists of a succession of phases in acquiring new linguistic forms. 3.4. The Influence of the Mother Tongue The widely held belief in the 1950s and 1960s was that the L1 played a decisive negative role in SLA, termed interference, and that this interference could be predicted by systematically comparing and contrasting the learner´s L1 and L2.

Transfer would be then a strategy available to compensate lack of L2 knowledge, being this lack supplied by terms adopted from L1. It can lead to errors, overproduction and constraints on hypotheses; besides similarities between native and target languages tend to cause many problems, namely false friends etc.

4.

The Treatment of Error

4.1. Introduction Before introducing the concept of error, we have to regard that for the study of SLA contrastive analyses have been used. They were motivated by the prospect of being able to identify points of similarity and difference between particular native languages (NLs) and target languages (TLs), believing that a more effective pedagogy would result when these were taken into consideration. A linguist called Lado justified the usage of the contrastive analysis in his work of 1957, he stated that individuals tend to transfer the forms and meanings and the distribution of forms and meanings of their native language culture to the foreign culture when attempting to speak the language and to act in the target culture. So, any researcher should be familiarised with the contrastive analysis since the major part of errors come from the interference that the native language causes in the production of a second language. 4.2.

Types of Errors Corder (1967) made a very interesting distinction between mistakes and

errors. Whereas a mistake is a random performance slip caused by fatigue, excitement, etc., and therefore can be readily self-corrected, an error is a systematic deviation made by learners who have not yet mastered the rules of the L2. In the following schema we are stating some of the most typical errors uttered by Spanish speakers when using English as a second language.

TYPE OR ERROR Interlingual: Interference Intralingual: Overgeneralization

Communication- Based

EXPLANATION

Is the book of my friend

The omission of the subject pronoun and the use of the “of the” possessive appear to be due to Spanish interference.

wonder where are you going

The speaker has perhaps overgeneralized the rule of subject – auxiliary inversion and applied it here

The learner uses “airball” for balloon

She cries as if the baby cries; FOR: She cries as a baby

Induced errors

4.3.

EXAMPLE

The learner incorrectly labels an object but successfully communicates a desired concept (“word coinage”)

The teacher had given the student a definition of “as if” meaning “like” without explaining the necessary structural change.

Hierarchy of Difficulty Stockwell´s et al. (1965) established a hierarchy of difficulty according to

acquisition of English by Spanish speakers regarding all those concepts that could be graded according to the difficulty they present.

The following table represents the

basis of Stockwell´s work. TYPE 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 4.4.

OF DIFICULTY Split New Absent Coalesced Correspondence

L1 SPANISH; L2 ENGLISH x----x/y 0----x x----0 x/y----x x-----x

EXAMPLE su----his/her possessive>s adjective plural morpheme por/para----for -ndo ---- -ing

Correction During the stage when students are asked to repeat and practise a certain

number of models, there are two basic correction stages: showing incorrectness (indicating to the student that something is wrong) and using correction techniques. 4.4.1

Showing Incorrectness This means that we will indicate to the student that a mistake has been

made. If the student understands this feedback, he or she will be able to correct

the mistake and this self-correction will be helpful as part of the learning process. There are a number of techniques showing incorrectness: 1)

Repeating: We simply ask the student to repeat what she or he has just said by using the word “again” with a questioning intonation, it indicates that the answer has been unsatisfactory although it could be misunderstood as only indicating that the teacher has not heard the student´s response.

2)

Echoing: We will be even clearer if we repeat what the student has just said, using a questioning intonation, since this will clearly indicate that we are doubting the accuracy or content of what is being said. Sometimes we can echo the complete student response, probably stressing the part of the utterance that was incorrect. Another possibility is to echo the student´s response, but only up to the point where the mistake was made. Echoing in this way is probably the most efficient way of showing incorrectness.

3)

Denial: We can simply tell the student that the response was unsatisfactory and ask for it to be repeated. It may be a bit more discouraging.

4)

Questioning: We can say: “Is that correct?” asking any student in the class to answer our question. This has the advantage of focusing everybody´s mind on the problem, though it may make the student who made the mistake seemed somewhat exposed.

5)

Expression: Many teachers indicate that a response was incorrect by their expression or by some gesture. Economical and also funny but can be dangerous if the student thinks that it is a form of mockery.

For showing incorrectness once the teacher has realized that the usage of correction techniques is necessary, tact and consideration are two features to be carefully taken into account. 4.4.2

Using Correction Techniques

If students are unable to correct themselves, we can resort to one of these techniques: 1) Student corrects student: We can ask if anyone else can give the correct response. We can ask if anyone can “help” the student who has made the mistake. If another student can supply the correct information, it will be good for that student´s self-esteem. However, the student who originally made the mistake may feel humiliated if this technique is used insensitively. 2) Teacher corrects student(s): Sometimes we may feel that we should take the charge of correction because the students are extremely mixed-up about what the correct response should be. In that case we can re-explain the item of language which is causing the trouble. This will be especially appropriate when we see that the majority of the class are having the same problem. After the re-explanation we can move to choral and individual repetition (if necessary) before moving on. It is important, therefore, that when we have used one of the techniques suggested above, we ask the student who originally made the mistake to give us a correct response. When students are involved in activities that demand some kind of creativity or in doing a drill-type activity (asking and answering some questions for example) and they commit an error another possibility of correction is seen, this is gentle correction. This involves showing the student that something is wrong, but not making a great fuss of it, and not asking for repetition. It also means telling the students what went right. Where they have achieved a successful outcome, or whether they have used good, appropriate language, they need to be told this. If so, the learning process will be one with low affective filter.

5.

Conclusion Acquiring a second language is a hard issue, and it has been demonstrated

in the theme that we have exposed. So, regarding our role as English teachers we

have to take into account the difficulties that have been realized during the exposition of the topic and deal with all the possible solutions that have also been exposed. First of all, it is very important to be acquainted with what teaching English as a second language consists of. This is the reason why different theories revolved around that fact have been proposed, dealing with nativist, environmentalist or interactionist models. It is important for the teacher to choose what is more relevant for him of them, taking into account all the actions that are going to be carried out by a teacher in order to fulfil some objectives, to know all what is essential about the background of these actions. And how them have been previously studied and worked upon by some other linguists in search of accuracy. Interlanguage is an important concept since it is recognizable in every stage of a learner, even the teacher will be in a stage of the interlanguage and has to facilitate the students to follow the convenient steps to be able to go across the different stages, once the teacher has been in that same stage of the interlanguage students are in this very moment, and once the teacher has been a student too. And finally the most important concern that has been stated in the theme regarding our role as teachers is the treatment of error, it comprehends a great deal on education, since capabilities of the student dealing with their affective status, and their comprehension of correctness towards error are completely linked with this point.